Chapter 5: The Developing Human

 

BRIEF CHAPTER OUTLINE

 

The Developing Fetus

            Stages of Prenatal Development

            Brain and Sensory Development Before Birth

            Environmental Influences on Detal Development

                        Maternal Nutrition

                        Teratogens

The Developing Infant and Child

            Physical Development in Infancy and Childhood

                        Early Motor Development

                        Early Sensory Development

                        Early Brain Development

Psychology in the Real World: Musical Training Changes the Brain

            Early Cognitive Development

                        Theory of Mind

            Development of Moral Reasoning

            Early Socioemotional Development

                        Attachment

                        Developing Social Relationships and Emotions

Breaking New Ground: How Touch and Comfort Influence Development

The Developing Adolescent

            Physical Development in Adolescence

            Cognitive and Brain Development

            Social Development in Adolescence

The Developing Adult

            Sensation and Perception in Adulthood

            Cognitive and Brain Development in Adulthood

            Cognitive Decline in the Aging Brain

            Socioemotional Development in Adults

            Death and Dying

Making Connections in Development: Personality Across the Life Span

Chapter Review


EXTENDED CHAPTER OUTLINE

 

  • Human development: the study of both change and continuity in the individual across the life span. This process begins before birth, in the prenatal environment of the mother’s womb.

 

THE DEVELOPING FETUS

  • We pass more biological milestones before birth than we will in the rest of our lives.

 

Stages of Prenatal Development

Three Stages:

1.   Germinal stage: begins at conception and lasts for two weeks.

  • Zygote: At conception, the fertilized egg forms a single-celled zygote.
  • By day 7 the multicelled organism is now called a blastocyst, which travels down the fallopian tube and attaches to the uterine wall.
  • Between 30% and 50% of the blastocysts do not attach properly and the pregnancy ends without the woman having known she was pregnant.
  • If implantation was successful, the second stage of prenatal development begins two weeks later.

2.   Embryonic stage: marked by the formation of the major organs: the nervous system, heart, eyes, ears, arms, legs, teeth, palate, and external genitalia. Embryonic development continues until about 8 weeks after conception.

  • Embryo: the bundle of rapidly multiplying cells (the blastocyst) that has implanted in the uterus.

3.   Fetal stage: the fetal stage is the formation of bone cells at 8 weeks after conception. By this time, all of the major organs have already begun to form. Between 8 and 12 weeks into development, the heartbeat can be detected with a stethoscope. During the fetal stage the organs continue to grow and mature while the fetus rapidly increases in size.

 

Brain and Sensory Development Before Birth

  • The first major organ to develop, the brain is still growing rapidly at birth.
  • By the time an infant is born, its head has grown to 25% of its adult weight, whereas its body is only 5% of its adult weight.
  • During the fetal stage, the rate of new neural growth can be approximately 3 million neurons per minute at its peak! From months 3 through 5 of pregnancy, neurons move from one part of the brain to their more permanent home in a process known as neural migration. 
  • Factors that interfere with migration include teratogens, such as prenatal exposure to
  • certain toxins or viruses, which can increase the risk of psychological disorders.
  • Generally, male fetuses are more active than females, suggesting their greater activity levels after birth may be inborn.

 

 

Environmental Influences on Fetal Development

  • What a pregnant mother eats, drinks, smokes, feels, and experiences plays an important role in fetal development.
  • Prenatal programming: the process by which events in the womb alter the development of physical and psychological health.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Chemical substances that a pregnant woman takes in or is exposed to shape the development of the fetus’s brain and other bodily systems.

  • For example, doctors prescribe folic acid and other vitamins to women who are pregnant or trying to become pregnant because they reduce the rates of abnormalities in the developing nervous system.

Teratogens

  • Teratogens: substances that can disrupt normal development and cause long-term effects. Examples include smoking, drinking alcohol, viruses, illness, chemicals, etc.
  • Because all major body parts are forming and growing during the embryonic and fetal stages, the fetus is quite susceptible to birth defects during these stages. Known teratogens include viruses, such as those that cause rubella (measles) and the flu; alcohol; nicotine; prescription drugs, such as the antidepressants Prozac and Zoloft; and radiation.
  • Timing determines how detrimental the effects of any given teratogen will be. In general, the earlier in pregnancy the woman is exposed, the more serious the effects.
  • Maternal substance use can also cause serious prenatal and postnatal problems. Pregnant women who drink alcohol take chances with their developing baby, as there is no known safe level of alcohol consumption during pregnancy – don’t drink!
  • Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD): causes damage to the central nervous system; low birth weight; physical abnormalities in the face, head, heart, and joints; mental retardation; and behavioral problems.
  • The effect of fetal alcohol exposure is described as a spectrum of disorders because the types and degrees of deficits can vary tremendously among individuals. FASD affects about 1% of live births in the United States and is a leading cause of mental retardation in this country.
  • FASD has been reported in babies of women who drink excessively as well as in infants whose mothers have only occasionally had drinks during pregnancy, although binge drinking and heavy drinking appear to increase the severity of FASD.
  • Smoking: Nicotine exposure from maternal smoking interferes with the oxygen supply to the fetus. It can lead to premature and low-birth-weight babies as well as increased risk for stillbirth (delivery of a dead fetus).
  • Prescription drugs: Some studies on animals and humans indicate that the antidepressants Zoloft and Prozac can cause respiratory problems, increased risk of premature birth, and short-lasting effects on motor development, but others suggest there are few risks to the developing fetus.
  • CONNECTION: Catching the flu virus while pregnant changes the way neurons grow in the developing fetus and increases vulnerability to schizophrenia later in life. (See Chapter 15.)

 

THE DEVELOPING INFANT AND CHILD

  • Because the brain is still developing immediately after a child is born, the environment the child is brought up in can shape it.
  • The newborn human brain is more responsive than that of other animals to the specific world it is in, allowing nurture to shape human nature more than is the case for most animals.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Because it is not well formed at birth, the newborn human brain is especially responsive to the specific world around it, allowing nurture to shape human nature.

 

Physical Development in Infancy and Childhood

Early Motor Development

  • When we speak of motor development, we are referring to changes in physical movement and body control.
  • Early in infancy, babies start to show intentional movements.
  • Then we see a fairly regular sequence: at 4 months, they can hold objects; at 6 months, many babies can sit by themselves; at 7 months, they move themselves around; at 8 to 9 months, babies start walking with assistance; and late in the first year many babies will take their first step. By 17 months, most babies walk with ease.

 

Early Sensory Development

  • Hearing is almost fully developed at birth, but a newborn’s vision is only about 20-600. Visual sharpness, or acuity, continues to improve during infancy, and by 6 months of age, vision is 20-100. By age 3 or 4, a child’s vision is similar to an adult’s.
  • Newborns are best able to see black and white edges and patterns. Color vision develops by around 6 months of age.
  • Experience is crucial in the development of vision, in regard to vision; the occipital cortex of the brain has to be stimulated by visual input in order to develop the proper synaptic connections needed to process visual information.
  • Critical period: specific period in biological development when individuals are most receptive to a particular kind of input from the environment (such as visual stimulation and language learning).
  • All babies who have normal vision in both eyes see the world in three dimensions. Soon after birth, they demonstrate the ability to detect depth in the real world.
  • The visual cliff: a test of depth perception in babies who have learned to crawl. Researchers placed clear Plexiglas over one end of a crawl area to make it look as though there was a steep drop in the middle of the crawl area. They put a baby on the other end of the crawl area and asked the mother to stand at the end with the drop. The mother’s role was to encourage the baby to crawl across the Plexiglas to her. The baby would stop crawling when he or she reached the visual cliff, indicating that at least by the time babies learn to crawl, they can perceive depth.

 

 

Early Brain Development

  • After birth, the brain continues to grow new neurons. By the second year of life, the human brain has more neurons than it will ever again have. Brain growth continues throughout the life span, but the rate of change slows down considerably after the age of 6 and then settles again after adolescence.
  • After age 2, some neurons and synapses die off. The reason for this is simple: During the first year of life, neural growth occurs, but it is somewhat random and disorganized. New neurons and synapses develop because that is what the newborn brain does.
  • With learning and experience certain synaptic connections become stronger, whereas those that do not receive stimulation from the environment die off. This process, known as pruning, is nature’s way of making the brain more efficient.
  • By adolescence, up to half of the synapses that existed in early childhood have been pruned.
  • CONNECTION: Experience is crucial in the formation of synaptic connections and the growth of neurons (neurogenesis) in the brain throughout the life span. Pruning is nature’s way of making the brain more efficient. (See Chapter 3.)

 

Psychology In The Real World: Musical Training Changes The Brain

  • The brain is most responsive to stimulation during infancy and childhood.
  • Early in life there is more opportunity for experience to leave its mark on the brain – for

       example, learning to play an instrument.

  • Researchers have found that for musicians, the somatosensory cortex shows lateralization that is not found in nonmusicians.
  • Musicians who started playing before the age of 12 show the most pronounced effects; so musical training may change brain organization, especially for people who start training as children.
  • Brain imaging studies also suggest that musical training molds the structure of the brain. People who have had intensive musical training have a thicker corpus callosum and increased brain growth in regions associated with music-related skills than do nonmusicians, even more so if they started their training before age 7.
  • This would mean that there is greater communication between the two sides of the brain in musicians than in people who have not had such training.
  • Also, musicians have larger cerebellums (an area involved in motor coordination) than do nonmusicians.
  • The findings discussed so far are correlational. They suggest that musical training can shape the brain, but do not lead to the conclusion that musical training causes brain growth.
  • To test the causal nature of this relationship, researchers taught a musical skill to one group and found that as skill improved, cortical representation for the finger muscles involved in the task increased.
  • They also found in subsequent research that practice has an effect. The brains of those who ceased practicing returned to the way they were previously. For those who continued practicing, brain map changes continued. If you don’t use it, you lose it!
  • Learning to play an instrument fosters the development of other skills too. Music training is positively correlated with intelligence test scores in children and college students, and this relationship is strongest for people who have trained longer.
  • It also improves performance on verbal memory tasks. Moreover, the ability to detect pitch changes in music aids processing of pitch changes in language processing.
  • Spelke (2008) found that children with more intensive musical training performed better on various spatial tasks, which is important in mathematics, a subject in which child musicians often excel.
  • Young brains are more flexible because they have less myelin. Myelination may close the window on the critical periods for such skills as learning language or music. In other words, brains are built in childhood, and those who wish to become musicians or athletes would do well to start young.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Musicians have better communication between the two sides of the brain than do people who have had no musical training.

 

Early Cognitive Development

·   With growth, especially brain growth, comes cognitive development – advances in the ability to think, reason, remember, learn, and solve problems.

Piaget’s 4 stages

1. Sensorimotor stage: Piaget’s first stage of cognitive development (ages 0–2), so called because infants learn about the world by using their senses and by moving their bodies in it.

·     One of the hallmarks of thinking at this age would be object permanence.

·    Object permanence: the ability to realize that objects still exist when they are not being sensed. Piaget argued this appears around 9 months of age. However, Baillargeon has found it in infants as young as 4 months.

2. Preoperational stage: the second major stage of cognitive development (ages 2–7), which begins with the emergence of symbolic thought, or the use of symbols such as words or letters to represent ideas or objects.

·     Symbolic thinking involves using symbols such as words or letters to represent ideas or objects. Other qualities of preoperational thinking include animistic thinking, egocentrism, and lack of conservation.

·     Animistic thinking: the idea that inanimate objects are alive.

·     Egocentrism: the tendency to view the world from one’s own perspective and not see things from another person’s perspective.

·     Conservation: the ability to recognize that when objects change shape or size, the overall amount stays the same.

3. Concrete operational stage: (ages 7–11) children can perform mental operations—on real, or concrete, objects and events—but they still have trouble with abstract ideas and reasoning.

· Reversing events is one type of operation a child masters in this stage.

4. Formal operational stage: During this stage, formal logic becomes possible. In addition, adolescents develop scientific reasoning and hypothesis-testing skills.

 

 

Theory of Mind

·         Theory of mind refers to our knowledge and ideas of how other people’s minds work. It involves knowing and understanding what other people are thinking, wanting, or feeling.

·         Children under the age of 4 do not realize that people may believe things that are not true. Adults know that people believe things—such as superstitions—that are untrue. Psychologists created the false-belief task to determine when children develop theory of mind and come to know that others can believe something that is false.

·         CONNECTION: Autism is marked by a lack of social interest as well as by heightened interest in some things and a tendency for counting things. How might autism be related to deficits in theory of mind?

 

Development of Moral Reasoning

·     Lawrence Kohlberg did for moral reasoning what Piaget did for cognitive development. Kohlberg (1981) studied the development of moral reasoning in children and adults by giving them a moral dilemma and recording the reasons they provided for their responses. Their responses were less important to him than the reasoning behind them.

·     Example, the dilemma of Heinz: “A woman was near death from a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the doctors thought might save her. It was a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was charging ten times what the drug cost him to produce. He paid $200 for the radium and charged $2,000 for a small dose of the drug. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he could only get together about $1,000, which is half of what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist said: ‘No, I discovered the drug and I’m going to make money from it.’ So Heinz got desperate and broke into the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife. Should Heinz have broken into the laboratory to steal the drug for his wife? Why or why not?” (Kohlberg, 1981).

·     Based on how people answered, he proposed a three-stage theory of moral reasoning. He found that moral reasoning moves from being focused on the self to being increasingly focused on others, with a basis in clear personal principles of morality and ethics.

1. Preconventional level: the first level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, in which moral reasoning involves avoiding punishment or maximizing rewards.

2. Conventional level: the second level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, during which the person values caring, trust, and relationships, as well as the social order and lawfulness.

3. Postconventional level: the third level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, in which the person acknowledges both the norm and the law, but argues that there are universal moral rules that may trump unjust or immoral local rules.

·   Research supports Kohlberg’s argument that children tend to reason preconventionally and adults conventionally. This is found cross-culturally, particularly for the first two stages.

·   The postconventional level appears to be limited more to Western cultures. When one realizes that Western cultures place a strong emphasis on individualism and individual values, this finding makes sense, because postconventional moral reasoning is heavily based in a personal moral code. In contrast, many non-Western cultures emphasize the group and community, and so the highest level of moral reasoning would be likely to involve compassion and caring for others, altruism, and family honor.

 

Early Socioemotional Development

         Attachment

·         Some animals, especially birds, follow and imitate the first large creature they see immediately after birth. This behavior is called imprinting. The newborn sees this creature as a protector. Usually this creature also happens to be the protector (mom or dad), so it is a good strategy. Newborn humans cannot follow around the first large creature they see, so they do not imprint. They attach.

·         Imprinting: the rapid and innate learning of the characteristics of a caregiver very soon after birth.

·         In everyday usage, attachment means “connectedness.” In human development, attachment refers to the strong emotional connection that develops early in life to keep infants close to their caregivers.

·         John Bowlby (1969) described how infants become emotionally attached to their caregivers and emotionally distressed when separated from them. He proposed that the major function of this affection-based bonding system is to protect infants from predation and other threats to survival.

·         In his observations of human infants and primates, Bowlby noted that they went through a clear sequence of reactions—from protest, to despair, to detachment—when separated from their caregiver.

·         Bowlby defined separation anxiety as the distress reaction shown by babies when they are separated from their primary caregiver (typically shown at around 9 months of age).

·         On the basis of such observations, Bowlby developed his attachment theory, which rests on two fundamental assumptions:

1. A responsive and accessible caregiver (usually the mother) must create a secure base for the child. The infant needs to know that the caregiver is accessible and dependable. With a dependable caregiver, the child can develop confidence and security in exploring the world.

2. Infants internalize the bonding relationship, which provides a mental model on which they build future friendships and love relationships. Therefore, attachment to a caregiver is the most critical of all relationships.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Human attachment is based on an affection-based bonding system that protects an infant from threats to survival.

·         Influenced by Bowlby’s work, Mary Ainsworth developed a technique for measuring the attachment of infant and caregiver. The strange situation task:  a 20-minute laboratory session in which a mother and her 12-month-old infant are initially alone in a playroom. Then a stranger comes into the room, and after a few minutes the stranger begins a brief interaction with the infant. The mother then leaves for two separate 2-minute periods. During the first period, the infant is left alone with the stranger. During the second period, the infant is left completely alone.

·         The critical behavior that Ainsworth and colleagues rated was how the infant reacted when the caregiver returned. They presumed that the infant’s reaction reflects the way the baby has learned to respond to his or her caregiver and that these reactions are based on the history of comfort and reassurance the caregiver has provided.

·         Based on this reunion behavior, Anisworth developed a classification system of three types:

1. Secure attachment, infants are happy and initiate contact when the mother returns. They will go over to her and want to be held. After they’ve been reunited with their mothers, they may return to their play.

·         The other two types of attachment represent insecure attachment.

2. Anxious-resistant attachment:  infants are ambivalent. When their mother leaves the room, they become unusually upset, and when their mother returns they seek contact with her but reject attempts at being soothed. These infants give very conflicted messages. On the one hand, they seek contact with their mother; on the other hand, they squirm to be put down and may throw away toys that their mother has offered them.

3. Anxious-avoidant attachment: infants stay calm when their mother leaves, they accept the stranger, and when their mother returns, they ignore and avoid her.

 

         Developing Social Relationships and Emotions

·         Developmental psychologist James Sorce studied one-year-old babies and their mothers’ reactions to the visual cliff. In his study, the mom would place her baby on the visual cliff. She would stand at the other end, put a toy down, and pose one of five facial expressions of emotion: fear, anger, sadness, interest, or happiness. She said nothing and did nothing else. When mom’s facial expression showed fear or anger, the baby did not move to the deep side. But most babies went willingly over the cliff when the mom smiled.

·         What this means is that by the age of one, children can make sense of their mothers’ emotional facial expressions and use them to know what to do. This ability to make use of social information from another person is known as social referencing.

·         CONNECTION: One way we learn is by imitating someone else’s behavior. Imitation also contributes to aggressive behavior.

·         All humans, including babies, respond to cues in their social environments. Exposure to aggressive conflict between parents, for example, changes babies’ behavior. Specifically, 6-month-old babies who have witnessed aggressive conflict between their parents tend to withdraw when presented with a novel stimulus, such as a new toy.

·         Emotional competence: the ability to control emotions and to know when it is appropriate to express certain emotions.

·         The development of emotional competence starts as early as preschool and continues throughout childhood. The better children do in school and the fewer stressful and dysfunctional situations they have at home, the more emotionally skilled and competent they are.

·         Peer interaction: As children get older, their social world expands from the intimate environment of the home to include play with other children. Although attachment to the primary caregiver is important for the baby and young child, relations with other children have a big impact after early childhood.

·         Nothing influences the behavior of children like other children – their peers.

·         Peers: persons who share equal standing or status.

 

Breaking New Ground: How Touch and Comfort Influence Development

·         See “Breaking New Ground” section for detailed explanation.

 

THE DEVELOPING ADOLESCENT

·         Adolescence: the transition period between childhood and adulthood, beginning at about age 11 or 12 and lasting until around age 18.

 

Physical Development in Adolescence

·             Puberty: the period when sexual maturation begins, marking the beginning of adolescence.

·             During puberty, major hormonal changes prepare the body for reproduction. On average, girls reach puberty at about age 11and boys at about age 13.

·             The beginning of puberty stems from the release of sex hormones.

·             First, the pituitary gland sends hormonal signals to the sex glands, telling them to mature. The sex glands, or gonads, then release sex hormones.

·             The male gonads are called testes; they release the male sex hormone testosterone,

·             The female gonads are the ovaries; they release estradiol.

·             In girls, breast development can start as early as age 10. The next major change is the onset of menstruation, known as menarche. The age of menarche is highly variable, but it often occurs by age 12. In most Western cultures, the age of menarche has dropped from about age 16 during the 1800s to 12 or 13 today.

·             In boys, the event that signals readiness to reproduce is spermarche, or the first ejaculation.

 

Cognitive and Brain Development

·             During adolescence, children gain the ability to reason about abstract concepts and problems. This is the stage of cognitive development that Piaget termed the formal operational stage.

·             In this stage, teens may show the ability to engage in scientific reasoning and hypothesis testing. Adolescents and even adults do not all develop this reasoning ability to the same degree.

·             The extent to which people develop scientific reasoning skills is related to their ability to think and solve problems systematically, rather than relying on the trial-and-error method that children use. It is also related to the ability to distinguish one’s thoughts about how the world works from the evidence for how it really works.

·             Neuroscientists have only recently uncovered how changes in thinking correspond with changes in the adolescent brain. Indeed, many of the cognitive developments of adolescence, such as abstract reasoning and logical thinking, may be a consequence of brain development.

·             In particular, the last part of the brain to fully develop—the frontal lobes—continues to mature until late adolescence or early adulthood.

·             The frontal lobes are involved in planning, attention, working memory, abstract thought, and impulse control. It is not so much that the frontal lobes are growing in size as that they are growing in complexity.

·             Specifically, the adolescent brain develops more myelin around the axons as well as more neural connections.

·             Myelination proceeds from the back of the brain to the front, where the frontal lobes are, during the period from childhood to adolescence.

·             The onset of formal operational and scientific thinking occurs after the frontal lobes have developed more fully.

·             CONNECTION: Can Internet gaming and alternative realities and personalities (avatars) be an addiction for some people?  (See Chapter 15.)

·             What effect does brain development have on intelligence? Researchers have known for decades that overall brain size is not correlated with overall intelligence. As it turns out, however, intelligence does seem to be associated with how the brain develops and, in particular, how the cortex develops.

·             At age 7 the highly intelligent children had thinner frontal cortexes, but by mid-adolescence their cortexes had become thicker than those of the children of average intelligence. Moreover, by age 19 the thickness of the cortex in the two groups was the same. These results suggest that the brains of highly intelligent people are more elastic and plastic and trace a different developmental path.

 

Social Development in Adolescence

·             An important part of social development in adolescence is the search for identity.

·             Puberty brings profound changes not only in the body but also in relationships. Family becomes less central, and peer and sexual relationships become paramount. Having close, intimate friends during adolescence is associated with many positive social and emotional outcomes, such as self-confidence, better relationships with parents and authority figures, and better performance in school.

·             Compared to childhood, the most obvious change in adolescent social development is the emergence of sexual interest and sexual relationships. Teens not only become interested in sexual relationships, but sexual thoughts and feelings also occupy much of their attention and time.

·             The average age for first sexual intercourse for men and women is around 17 years old, although there is quite a bit of variability in when people start having sex.

 

THE DEVELOPING ADULT

·             As people enter their 30s, development has largely stabilized.

 

Sensation and Perception in Adulthood

·             Many people experience some loss of vision or hearing or both by middle adulthood.

·             Hearing declines, too, with age. A recent large-scale study found that as many as 50% of older adults (mean age of 67) experience some degree of hearing loss.

·             CONNECTION: Mosquito ring tones for cell phones were developed by young people to exploit older adults’ decreasing ability to hear high-pitched sounds. (See Chapter 4.)

·             Some people also experience a loss of sensitivity to taste and smell, though these changes vary considerably among individuals.

·             Losing one’s sense of smell can dampen the sense of taste to the point that food no longer has much appeal, somewhat like what happens when you have a bad cold. As many as half the people over 65 demonstrate significant loss of smell.

 

Cognitive and Brain Development in Adulthood

·             The older brain does not change as rapidly as the younger brain. Yet new experiences and mastery of new skills continue to give rise to neural branching and growth throughout life.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Mastering new skills stimulates neural growth and the formation of new synapses throughout the life span.

 

Healthy Aging

·             Aging is not a slow decline over time (Kenyon, 2005). Keeping the mind active increases neural branching, which in turn improves cognitive performance.

·             For optimal aging, we can engage in some healthy activities. “Use it or lose it” is one approach to healthy aging. Learning new skills, such as a new language, a new game, or a new computer activity can lead to new neural growth.

 

Cognitive Decline in the Aging Brain

·             Normal changes in the brain occur with age. We used to think that the brain lost cells as part of normal aging. This appears to be an overstatement. Just as body mass gradually decreases with age, so too does brain mass.

·             Dementia: a loss of cognitive functions, including memory problems and difficulty reasoning, solving problems, making decisions, and using language. Several neurological conditions, including stroke and Alzheimer’s disease, can lead to dementia in the elderly.

·             Alzheimer’s disease: a degenerative disease marked by progressive cognitive decline and characterized by a collection of symptoms, including confusion, memory loss, mood swings, and eventual loss of physical function.

·             Alzheimer’s accounts for 60–70% of the cases of dementia among the elderly.

·             Some evidence suggests that neurogenesis in the adult brain might offset or even prevent the kind of neural degeneration seen in Alzheimer’s and other age-related brain disorders.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Aerobic exercise appears to protect against a decline in higher mental processing among aging adults and may actually make the brain grow.

 

Socioemotional Development in Adults

·             In spite of the dramatic changes that can occur in cognitive functioning, there are fewer age-related changes in emotional functioning.

·             According to socioemotional selectivity theory, as people age and become more aware of their limited time, they become more selective about where they expend their resources in personal and emotional relationships. They waste less time in superficial acquaintances and invest more in sustaining meaningful, fulfilling connections in their lives.

 

Death and Dying

·             In psychological terms, death is a complex event that marks the end of life.

 

Making Connections in Development: Personality Across the Life Span

  • See “Making the Connections” section for detailed explanation.

 

KEY TERMS

 

adolescence: the transition period between childhood and adulthood.

Alzheimer’s disease: a degenerative disease marked by progressive cognitive decline and characterized by a collection of symptoms, including confusion, memory loss, mood swings, and eventual loss of physical function.

animistic thinking: belief that inanimate objects are alive.

anxious-avoidant: attachment style characterized by infants who stay calm when their primary caregiver leaves and who ignore and avoid her when she returns.

anxious-resistant attachment: attachment style characterized by infants who are ambivalent when separated and reunited.

attachment: the strong emotional connection that develops early in life between infants and their caregivers.

concrete operational stage: Piaget’s third stage of cognitive development, which spans ages

6–11, during which the child can perform mental operations—such as reversing—on real objects or events.

conservation: recognition that when some properties (such as shape) of an object change, other properties (such as volume) remain constant.

conventional level: the second level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, during which the person values caring, trust, and relationships as well as the social order and lawfulness.

critical period: specific period in development when individuals are most receptive to a particular kind of input from the environment (such as visual stimulation and language).

dementia: a loss of mental function, in which many cognitive processes are impaired, such as the ability to remember, reason, solve problems, make decisions, and use language. 

egocentrism: viewing the world from one’s own perspective and not being capable of seeing things from another person’s perspective.

embryo: the term for the developing organism from 2 weeks until about 8 weeks after conception.

embryonic stage: the second prenatal stage, from 2 weeks to 8 weeks after conception, when all of the major organs form.

emotional competence: the ability to control emotions and know when it is appropriate to express certain emotions.

fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD): a consequence of prenatal alcohol exposure that causes multiple problems, notably brain damage and mental retardation.

fetal stage: the third prenatal stage, which begins with the formation of bone cells 8 weeks after conception and ends at birth.

formal operational stage: Piaget’s fi nal stage of cognitive development, from age 11 or 12 on through adulthood, when formal logic is possible.

generativity: a term Erik Erikson used to describe the process in adulthood of creating new ideas, products, or people.

germinal stage: the first pre natal stage of development, which begins at conception and lasts two weeks.

human development: the study of change and continuity in the individual across the life span.

imprinting: the rapid and innate learning of the characteristics of a caregiver very soon after birth.

menarche:  the first menstrual period.

neural migration: the movement of neurons from one part of the fetal brain to their more permanent destination; occurs during months 3–5 of the fetal stage.

object permanence: the ability to realize that objects still exist when they are not being sensed.

peers: people who share equal standing or status and are at the same level, in terms of age, gender, skill, or power.

preconventional level: the first level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, focusing on avoiding punishment or maximizing rewards.

prenatal programming: the process by which events in the womb alter the development of physical and psychological health.

preoperational stage: the second major stage of cognitive development (ages 2–5), which begins with the emergence of symbolic thought.

postconventional level: the third level in Kohlberg’s theory of moral reasoning, in which the person recognizes universal moral rules that may trump unjust or immoral local rules.

pruning: the degradation of synapses and dying off of neurons that are not strengthened by experience.

puberty: the period when sexual maturation begins; it marks the beginning of adolescence.

secure attachment: attachment style characterized by infants who will gradually explore new situations when the caregiver leaves, and they initiate contact when the caregiver returns after separation.

sensorimotor stage: Piaget’s fi rst stage of cognitive development (ages 0–2), when infants learn about the world by using their senses and by moving their bodies.

separation anxiety: the distress reaction shown by babies when they are separated from their primary caregiver (typically shown at around 9 months of age).

social referencing: the ability to make use of social and emotional information from another person—especially a caregiver—in an uncertain situation.

spermarche: the first ejaculation.

temperament: the biologically based tendency to behave in particular ways from very early in life.

teratogens: substances that can disrupt normal prenatal development and cause lifelong deficits.

theory of mind: ideas and knowledge about how other people’s minds work.

zygote: the single cell that results when a sperm fertilizes an egg.

 

MAKING THE CONNECTIONS

 

Teratogens

CONNECTION: Catching the flu virus while pregnant changes the way neurons grow in the developing fetus and increases vulnerability to schizophrenia later in life. (See Chapter 15.)

 

Early Brain Development

CONNECTION: Experience is crucial in the formation of synaptic connections and the growth of neurons (neurogenesis) in the brain throughout the life span. Pruning is nature’s way of making the brain more efficient. (See Chapter 3.)

o       Discussion: This is a good time to discuss the concept of epigenisis.  A great site on pruning is: http://faculty.washington.edu/chudler/plast.html. I typically show clips from the PBS series “The Secret Life of the Brain”: An overview of the series: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/teenbrain/work/adolescent.html and the video links: the link for the infant: http://www.pbs.org/wnet/brain/episode1/video.html, the child: http://www.pbs.org/wnet/brain/episode2/video.html, and the teenager: http://www.pbs.org/wnet/brain/episode3/video.html.

 

Theory of Mind

CONNECTION: Autism is marked by a lack of social interest as well as by heightened interest in some things and a tendency for counting things. How might autism be related to deficits in theory of mind?

  • Discussion: Have students read http://www.iidc.indiana.edu/irca/education/TheoryofMind.html before coming to class. Ask them to write down 3-4 examples they have seen in their lives of theory of mind (or lack thereof) in children. Begin the discussion by asking them to provide examples and then discuss what it would be like without that knowledge in adulthood. For example, in my family my younger brother would hit himself and yell that one of us had hit him – the noise with the accusation was always enough for someone to get into trouble. This demonstrates the utility of theory of mind.

 

Developing Social Relationships and Emotions

CONNECTION: One way we learn is by imitating someone else’s behavior. Imitation also contributes to aggressive behavior.

o       Discussion: This is a great time to discuss social learning theory (see Chapter 8 on Learning). If you are not prepared to go into discussing Bandura’s work on media effects on aggressive behavior in depth, it still can be a useful example of modeling and imitation.

 

Cognitive and Brain Development

CONNECTION: Can Internet gaming and alternative realities and personalities (avatars) be an addiction for some people? (See Chapter 15.)

  • Discussion: Have students take the Internet Addiction Test (IAT): http://www.netaddiction.com/resources/internet_addiction_test.htm. Ask them if they feel that people can really be addicted to the Internet. What about their Blackberrys? Although they may be in a younger cohort, point out that the underlying issue here may not be checking Facebook over email but checking over conversations in real life. Ask students about other people’s behaviors. How often are they on the phone or Internet while sitting with someone else?

 

Sensation and Perception in Adulthood

CONNECTION: Mosquito ring tones for cell phones were developed by young people to exploit older adults’ decreasing ability to hear high-pitched sounds. (See Chapter 4.)

 

Making Connections in Development: Personality Across the Life Span

  • One thing that stays with us our entire lives is our personality. It may change a bit, but for the most part personality tends to be a fairly stable part of who we are.
  • Temperament: The biologically based tendency to behave in specific ways. It makes up the building blocks of personality.
  • Personality: the consistently unique way in which an individual behaves over time and situations.

 

Temperament and Personality in the Fetus and Infant

  • After birth, some infants soon settle into a predictable routine. Others do not. Some are generally happy, and others aren’t. And some infants have lower thresholds for stimulation than others.
  • Thomas and Chess developed a classification of three types of personality based on differences in temperament.

1. Easy: predictable in daily functions, is happy most of the time, and is adaptable. About 40% of children fall into this category.

2. Difficult: unpredictable in daily functions, is unhappy most of the time, and is slow to adapt to new situations. About 10% fall into this category.

3. Slow-to-warm-up: mildly intense in his or her reactions to new situations and mildly irregular in the daily patterns of eating, sleeping, and eliminating. Although his or her first response to new situations might be negative, after repeated exposures, he or she develops an approaching style. About 15% of the children fall into this category.

  • These three dimensions do not classify about 35% of children.

 

Childhood Temperament and Personality

  • One longitudinal study evaluated 1,000 New Zealand children on many temperamental, cognitive, medical, and motor dimensions at age 3 and then again about every 2 to 2.5 years until they were 21 years old.
  • Ratings by parents at age 3 revealed three basic types of temperament: well-adjusted, undercontrolled, and inhibited.
  • Eighteen years after the initial assessment, the individuals whose parents had classified them as “undercontrolled” (impulsive and prone to temper tantrums) at age 3, were impulsive and likely to engage in thrill-seeking behaviors, more likely to be aggressive and hostile, to have more relationship conflict, and to abuse alcohol.
  • At age 21, “inhibited” children were less likely to have social support and were more likely to avoid risk and harm, to be nonassertive and overcontrolled, and to suffer from prolonged depression.

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Infants born to mothers who experienced an unusual amount of stress during pregnancy tend to be more sensitive to stress throughout childhood and beyond.

 

Adolescent and Adult Personality Development

  • Erikson proposed a model of personality development with eight stages, each defined by an identity crisis or conflict.
  • An identity crisis is an opportunity for adaptive or maladaptive adjustment. Each stage consists of a conflict from which a person may develop a strength.
  • Identity versus identity confusion: the conflict during adolescence.  Testing, experimenting, and trying on identities is the norm during adolescence.
  • For early adulthood—the period during one’s 20s—the conflict is between intimacy and isolation. Erikson defined intimacy as the ability to fuse one’s identity with another’s without the fear of losing it.
  • In adulthood, the stage that lasts from about 30 to 60 or 65 years of age, the conflict is what Erikson called generativity versus stagnation.
  • Generativity: the creation of new ideas, products, or people.
  • Stagnation: when the adult becomes more self-focused than oriented toward others and does not contribute in a productive way to society or family.
  • The old age stage, starting around age 60 or 65. The conflict of old age is between integrity and despair. Integrity is the feeling of being whole and integrated. It is the sense that all of one’s life decisions are coming together and make sense.
  • Discussion: This is a good time to discuss “goodness of fit.” Or the idea that the temperament of the child in combination with the temperament of parent is where the real action is. In other words,  a difficult child with a difficult parent could be far more problematic than if he or she were paired with an easy parent. 

 

NATURE-NURTURE POINTERS

 

Environmental Influences on Fetal Development

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Chemical substances that a pregnant woman takes in or is exposed to shape the development of the fetus’s brain and other bodily systems.

  • Discussion: A great example here is fetal alcohol syndrome – in some cases, consuming alcohol while pregnant leads to a misshapen head and a slew of cognitive deficits. Another could be smoking, which leads to low birth weight and thus cognitive deficits.

 

The Developing Infant and Child

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Because it is not well formed at birth, the newborn human brain is especially responsive to the specific world around it, allowing nurture to shape human nature.

  • Discussion: Again, you may want to stress the idea of epigenisis. This is also a good time to point out that Piaget’s theory is epigenetic in nature. That is, he believes that children are active and cause their own development as they interact with the world. Thus an impoverished environment (one with little chance for exploration – for example, with the baby stuck in a playpen or bounce seat all day) should lead to limited cognitive development; and an environment that has many opportunities for exploration should lead to a more complex level of thought.

 

Psychology in the Real World: Music Lessons May Make Children Smarter

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Musicians have better communication between the two sides of the brain than do people who have had no musical training.

  • Discussion: You may want to have students read: http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/26388.php and then a BBC article on a disorder that strikes musicians to a greater extent than nonmusicians at: http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/health/3490158.stm. Ask students what they think about these lines of research that show both advantages and disadvantages of musical training. Also discuss what they think the reasons for these results are. Ask students how many of them took music lessons and for how long. Do they think they have advantages because of it?

 

Attachment

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Human attachment is based on an affection-based bonding system that protects an infant from threats to survival.

Discussion:  This is a great time to discuss the evolutionary adaptive value of imprinting and attachment. Here is a cute clip of ducks following a canoe: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ySvDoV5ZRzE. You may want to tie this in to how infant attachment most likely operates on a similar level. You could also relate this to Harlow and his monkeys showing autistic behaviors when left in isolation. 

 

Cognitive and Brain Development in Adulthood

Nature-Nurture Pointer: Mastering new skills stimulates neural growth and the formation of new synapses throughout the life span.

o  Discussion: Remind students that the brain never stops developing. All interactions and knowledge change the way the brain works. Repeated information and exposure tends to strengthen neural networks and allow for more connected thinking.

 

Cognitive Decline in the Aging Brain

Nature Nurture Pointer: Aerobic exercise appears to protect against a decline in higher mental processing among aging adults and may actually make the brain grow.

  • Discussion: Remind students that exercise releases neurotransmitters that make you feel good, hence an exercise high of sorts. Also, we see that individuals with poor health tend to show greater cognitive declines. So anything that aids you in being in good health seems to also help keep you in good cognitive functioning. Check out http://www.fi.edu/learn/brain/exercise.html for more information.  

 

Breaking New Ground: How Touch and Comfort Influence Development

·         One of the surest ways to soothe a crying newborn is to pick her up and cuddle her close. Aside from its calming effect, how important is contact for a developing infant?  And what purpose does it serve?

 

Do Babies’ Needs Extend Beyond Hunger and Thirst?

·         In the early part of the 20th century, psychologists assumed that all babies needed to survive was to have their internal biological needs met – hunger, thirst, and temperature regulation.

·         External needs, including love, affection, and social contact, were considered secondary. According to this view, babies liked being held by their mothers because they had come to associate mom with the ability to satisfy their primary hunger needs.

 

The importance of Comfort and Physical Contact

·         Harry Harlow thought there might be more to infants’ desire for contact than a need for nourishment. In his early work, Harlow (1958) noticed that baby monkeys whom he had separated from their mothers became very attached to cloth diapers that lined their cages. This strong attachment to cloth made Harlow think that a baby primate needs something soft to cling to. It reminded him of the attachment babies have for their blankets.

·         To test his hunch, Harlow and his colleagues carried out a series of studies with newborn monkeys whom they separated from their mothers. They housed them with surrogate mothers constructed of wire and wood. One was just a wire frame with a crude head. The other was a wire frame covered with soft terry cloth. Both mothers were heated and either could be hooked up to a bottle of milk.

·         In the first study, Harlow removed eight monkeys from their mothers shortly after birth. Cloth and wire mothers were housed in cubicles attached to the infants’ cages. Half the monkeys were randomly assigned to get milk from the wire monkey; the other half got their milk from the cloth monkey.

·         Harlow used the amount of time spent with a surrogate mother as a measure of the affection bond. He found that contact comfort was much more important than the source of food in determining which surrogate mother the monkeys preferred. Regardless of whether a baby monkey nursed from the cloth mother or the wire mother, it spent most of its time with the cloth mom.

·         Harlow’s findings suggested that the view that babies preferred being with their moms because the mothers provided food was at least partially incorrect. Harlow went so far as to say that a primary function of nursing in humans was contact as much as nutrition.

·         Field and her colleagues (1986) decided to test whether regular touch might help tiny premature infants. She randomly assigned 40 preterm infants from a hospital’s newborn intensive care unit to either receive touch therapy (experimental group) or not (control group). All of the premature infants lived in isolettes, plastic-covered bassinets designed to prevent infection. This touch therapy involved gently stroking the baby with warmed hands (no gloves) through portholes in the isolette for 15 minutes, three times a day for 10 days.

·         Over the treatment period, babies who received touch therapy gained significantly more weight than those who did not, even though they did not eat more.

·         Later research showed the same effect in weight gain when mothers touched their preterm infants. Touch also leads to reduced stress levels in premature babies and to fewer diarrheas. So touch in this case, makes for better health!

 

Extending the Benefits of Touch Therapy

·         Massage therapy also helps people with developmental disorders, such as autism. A study of Qigong (a Chinese touch therapy) showed improvements in sensory, social, and basic living skills in autistic children.

·         Massage improves well-being, motor dexterity, sleeping, and overall health in kids with other disabilities.

 

 

INNOVATIVE INSTRUCTION

 

Additional Discussion Topics

  1. Gender and Moral Development: Gilligan argues that psychology has underestimated sex differences in development and thinking. Specifically, she argued that the traditional view of moral development (Kohlberg’s) was unfair to women. She argued for a “caring” vs. “justice” orientation as opposed to the Kohlbergian view. For more information, see http://www.stolaf.edu/people/huff/classes/handbook/Gilligan.html.
  2. Piaget: This is a great time to discuss the educational implications of Piaget’s work. Remind students that Piaget felt that peers only offered a state of disequilibrium and that was the only benefit. You may want to tie in Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development as a counter-perspective to this. Also point out to students that Piaget believed children cause their own development. Montessori based her educational work on this perspective. You can show Montessori and her work: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=748019594080989064&hl=en
  3. Harlow: Start by showing a clip of Harlow’s work: http://video.google.com/videosearch?q=Harlow&emb=0#emb=0&q=harry%20harlow%20monkey&src=3 & http://video.google.com/videosearch?q=Harlow&emb=0# & http://video.google.com/videosearch?q=Harlow&emb=0#emb=0&q=harry%20harlow%20monkey&src=3.
  4. Pruning: Show students: http://www.redorbit.com/news/video/science/5/human_brain_goes_through_growth_spurts_and_pruning/22400/index.html. Have students write a short paragraph on the many different types of applications research into proteins could have.
  5. Teratogens: Have students do a web search for different teratogens and bring a list to class. Then have students volunteer information they found. Some of this is obvious (FAS, smoking, crack, etc.); however, some of the things like lunch meat may lead to a lively discussion.

 

Activities

  1. Download the Kohlberg dilemmas from: http://www.haverford.edu/psych/ddavis/p109g/kohlberg.dilemmas.html. Place students in small 4-5-person groups and pass out different dilemmas to each group. Give students 20 minutes to read the scenario and fill out the questions according to each of the three levels of thinking.  Then have each group provide a brief 5-minute synopsis to the class of their dilemma and how each level would respond.
  2. Have students ask their parents what they were like as a infant and child and write two paragraphs – one reporting what their parents said and the second on how it maps onto they way they are today. That is, in their case was personality stable over time?
  3. Have students look at http://www.psych.uiuc.edu/~rcfraley/attachment.htm, which has a great overview of how infant attachment correlates with adult attachment styles. Then have them go to http://www.web-research-design.net/cgi-bin/crq/crq.pl to take a quick survey on adult attachment style (it even plots where they are on the graph). Then have them write a paragraph summarizing infant attachment styles, a paragraph on adult attachment styles, and finally a paragraph on if they feel this assumption of adult effects is correct, based on their score.  
  4. This is a nice tie-in with the “Psychology in the Real World” section of the text. Have students read “Music Lessons Enhance IQ”: http://www.psychologicalscience.org/pdf/ps/musiciq.pdf, and the article at  http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2006/09/060920093024.htm  and write a brief 2-paragraph summary of the research described on the relationship between music and intelligence.
  5. Have students complete the worksheet on Piaget’s stages at: http://psychology.about.com/library/quiz/bl_piaget_quiz.htm. Review with students that these stages are integral to Piaget’s work. You may also want to point out that Piaget’s stages revolve around being able to think a certain way – not necessarily the age itself. That said, Piaget firmly believed that chronological years were important, as that should dictate the amount of experience.

 

Suggested Films

1.      An interview with David Elkind discussing Piaget: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=-9014865592046332725

2.      A brief clip on Vygotsky’s theory: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=634376752589779456&hl=en

3.      A Vygotskyan approach to play: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=36813446619688577&hl=en

4.      A brief overview of Mary Ainsworth and attachment: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=-3634664472704568591

5.      The strange situation task: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=-3479676835864606277&hl=en

6.      Erik Erikson’s work: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=-7953598721199398444&hl=en

7.      Montessori and her work: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=748019594080989064&hl=en

8.      Bandura’s Social Cognitive theory: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=-2953790276071699877&hl=en

9.      Cute clip of a conservation task: http://video.google.com/videoplay?docid=7120969848896411546&vt=lf&hl=en

10.  NOVA clips on life’s greatest miracle: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/miracle/program.html

11.  Martin Seligman on positive psychology: http://www.ted.com/index.php/talks/martin_seligman_on_the_state_of_psychology.html

 

Suggested Websites

1.      The student page is great as a resource of Piaget’s work: http://www.piaget.org/

2.      Educational implications of Piaget’s theory: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science?_ob=ArticleURL&_udi=B6VD4-40V4CM7-8&_user=10&_rdoc=1&_fmt=&_orig=search&_sort=d&view=c&_version=1&_urlVersion=0&_userid=10&md5=9b8ba4e000130888c49d5efa3c628b7f

3.      A great site on cognitive development: http://cogweb.ucla.edu/

4.      Piaget’s website: http://www.piaget.org/

5.      An overview of Piaget’s theory: http://chiron.valdosta.edu/whuitt/col/cogsys/piaget.html

6.      An overview of Erikson’s theory: http://chiron.valdosta.edu/whuitt/col/affsys/erikson.html

7.      An overview of Kohlberg’s stages: http://psychology.about.com/od/developmentalpsychology/a/kohlberg.htm

8.      An overview of Harlow’s work: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/aso/databank/entries/bhharl.html

9.      A quiz on Piaget’s stages: http://psychology.about.com/library/quiz/bl_piaget_quiz.htm

10.  Some great psychology tests and quizzes, including excerpts from the Hazan and Shaver adult attachment inventory: http://psychology.about.com/lr/psychology_quizzes/2370/3/

11.  An overview of Ainsworth’s work: http://www.enotes.com/psychology-theories/ainsworth-mary-d-salter

12.  Another Kohlberg page: http://www.vtaide.com/blessing/Kohlberg.htm

13.  A great Piaget page: http://www.learningandteaching.info/learning/piaget.htm

14.  A great overview of developmental theories: http://tigger.uic.edu/~lnucci/MoralEd/overview.html

 

Suggested Readings

Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M.C., Waters, M., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Beilen, H. (1992). Piaget’s enduring contribution to developmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 28, 191-204.

Bruner, J. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth: A collaboration at the Center for Cognitive Studies. New York: Wiley & Sons.

Elkind, D. (1988). The Hurried Child: Growing Up Too Fast Too Soon. New York: Addison-Wesley.

Erikson, E. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: Norton.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Kuhn, D., Langer, J., Kohlberg, L., & Haan, N. S. (1977). The development of formal operations in logical and moral judgment. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 97-188.

Piaget, J. (1990). The child’s conception of the world. New York: Littlefield Adams.

Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. The Free Press: New York.

Power, F. C., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1989). Lawrence Kohlberg's Approach to Moral Education. New York: Columbia University Press.

Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and Language. Boston: MIT Press.

Williams, G.C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.